In language, there's no such thing as a common error
This is a statement mired in controversy. It wasn't me that first said it, but I agree with it... with one caveat: we're talking about native language.
For many, many years, grammarians and school teachers would hound us for saying things wrong. As a child, I was constantly "corrected" by my mother for asking for permission with Can I...? instead of Please may I...? or for saying if I was you... in place of if I were you....
So when I studied English at university it was very heartening to find that modern linguistics considers everything that is said by a sizable chunk of the population as acceptable language. And of course this includes both Can I...? and if I was you....
What triggered this post was seeing an article on the Register about a grammatical error in a BBC headline: Phone-hacking: the other news you might of missed.
This is one of those "errors" that's now common enough and consistent enough that we may have to stop calling it an error.
When I suggest this, people often recoil in horror. "But it's the perfect tense," they cry, "logically it must be have." (And yes - I know that perfect is an aspect, not a tense, but pointing that out at this point would seem like cheap point-scoring so I generally let it lie.) But since when was language logical? You must and You have to are logically equivalent in some usage, but when you negate them you get two very different things: you mustn't and you don't have to.
The thing is, logic aside, we have empirical evidence that shows people's brains don't see it as have -- the errors themselves stand as proof of an emerging norm. Rather than fretting about the logic of have=perfect, we should be paying close attention to the thought processes behind this change and trying to make the way we right English match the way we speak it.
This does not mean that we have to accept might of, could of etc. No, because there is an existing mechanism that rids us of this problem: contractions.
Contractions are mostly hated by our schoolroom English teachers, but they are gaining growing acceptance. We're allowed can't now, where my primary school teacher insisted on cannot, and even I'm where my teacher insisted on I am. Yet we're still told off by teachers and editors if we try to use could've or coulda, should've or shoulda, might've or mighta. But these are what we say. Our habits of speaking have gradually reduced the auxiliary have to something more of a fusional element, a suffix, than a word. It is only when a writer is expected to write "in full words" that might've becomes might of, so why not simply accept might've? It would eliminate both the error and the controversy, and would say several pedants a few more grey hairs....
Showing posts with label what sounds right. Show all posts
Showing posts with label what sounds right. Show all posts
23 July 2011
20 May 2011
"Say what sounds right."
Bad advice has an annoying habit of sounding like good advice, and this little phrase really is something of a wolf in sheep's clothing. It's definitely appealling when someone points out that that's what we do in our own language.
I wouldn't argue that my end-goal isn't to be able to simply say what sounds right, but I just can't see how that end-goal affects my learning path: nothing will never "sound right" until I've learnt it, so how can I learn by what "sounds right"?
The consequences of this are not insubstantial, because if I've not learned it yet, what's going to sound right to me? What sounds right is something that I have learned, but this will be out of context.
Take for example the verb "start" in English.
You can "start something".
You can "start doing something".
You can "start with something".
You can "start by doing something".
If you learn only two of these, then only those two will "sound right".
Saying "what sounds right" traps you into what you know and stops you expanding your language. What you need to do is stop and think, and use the appropriate form, even if it isn't familiar enough to "sound right" yet.
Here's another example.
French has a feature called "liaison" -- certain final consonants are silent but reappear when followed by a vowel, but only if the two words are tied together syntactically.
In the word "vous", the S is normal silent, but in the phrase "vous allez" (you go), it has a /z/ sound.
Now, the past participle of to go is "allé", which is pronounced identically to "allez", so when you ask "êtes-vous allé" (have you gone), if you go by what "sounds right", you might pronounce that /z/. But in this question "vous" and "allé" are not syntactically bound together and liaison should not occur.
The idea of "what sounds right" reaches a very messy conclusion in Scottish Gaelic. A single syllable consisting of a schwa before a noun can be one of three things: "the", "his" or "her". "his" always causes initial lenition (soft mutation of the first consonant) of the following noun, "her" never does. As "the", this form can occur before masculine and feminine nouns in certain cases, and causes initial lenition, and only with certain letters.
Many teachers suggest that you learn noun gender by "what sounds right", by agreement with the article, but your ear will be exposed to the various case-inflected forms and possessives, so what sounds right might not be "the boy" at all, but "her boy" or "his boy".
Bad advice has an annoying habit of sounding like good advice, and this little phrase really is something of a wolf in sheep's clothing. It's definitely appealling when someone points out that that's what we do in our own language.
I wouldn't argue that my end-goal isn't to be able to simply say what sounds right, but I just can't see how that end-goal affects my learning path: nothing will never "sound right" until I've learnt it, so how can I learn by what "sounds right"?
The consequences of this are not insubstantial, because if I've not learned it yet, what's going to sound right to me? What sounds right is something that I have learned, but this will be out of context.
Take for example the verb "start" in English.
You can "start something".
You can "start doing something".
You can "start with something".
You can "start by doing something".
If you learn only two of these, then only those two will "sound right".
Saying "what sounds right" traps you into what you know and stops you expanding your language. What you need to do is stop and think, and use the appropriate form, even if it isn't familiar enough to "sound right" yet.
Here's another example.
French has a feature called "liaison" -- certain final consonants are silent but reappear when followed by a vowel, but only if the two words are tied together syntactically.
In the word "vous", the S is normal silent, but in the phrase "vous allez" (you go), it has a /z/ sound.
Now, the past participle of to go is "allé", which is pronounced identically to "allez", so when you ask "êtes-vous allé" (have you gone), if you go by what "sounds right", you might pronounce that /z/. But in this question "vous" and "allé" are not syntactically bound together and liaison should not occur.
The idea of "what sounds right" reaches a very messy conclusion in Scottish Gaelic. A single syllable consisting of a schwa before a noun can be one of three things: "the", "his" or "her". "his" always causes initial lenition (soft mutation of the first consonant) of the following noun, "her" never does. As "the", this form can occur before masculine and feminine nouns in certain cases, and causes initial lenition, and only with certain letters.
Many teachers suggest that you learn noun gender by "what sounds right", by agreement with the article, but your ear will be exposed to the various case-inflected forms and possessives, so what sounds right might not be "the boy" at all, but "her boy" or "his boy".
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)